We have completed maintenance on DiscoverMagazine.com and action may be required on your account. Learn More

Nutrition Science Has a Credibility Problem. We Might Blame Gut Microbes

Eat carbs. Don't eat carbs. Eat bacon. Don't eat bacon. What if the reason studies never agree is because people's gut microbes are so different?

The Crux
By Anna Funk
Oct 1, 2018 10:00 PMJul 28, 2020 1:01 AM
gut microbiome - shutterstock
(Credit: T. L. Furrer/Shutterstock)

Newsletter

Sign up for our email newsletter for the latest science news
 

After decades of research have yielded nothing but flip-flopping dietary advice sprinkled with the occasional scientific scandal, many health-conscious people are placing their trust elsewhere.

Just walk down any aisle of a Whole Foods to find health claims ranging from benignly naturopathic, to pseudo-scientific, to outright anti-scientific. It seems people would rather put faith in a fad diet promoted by a blogger, some guy with a book deal, or their cousin’s sister-in-law on Facebook than advice from doctors and nutrition researchers.

We can’t blame them. Just last month, one of the most prominent researchers in diet and nutrition, Brian Wansink, was removed from his positions at Cornell University after facing allegations of research misconduct. You might remember hearing that your plate size affects how full you feel, or that people duped with a refilling trick bowl eat way more soup than they do when they have to ask for a refill. That guy. As it turns out, some inconsistencies in Wansink’s published work revealed shady data practices. He wasn’t making up numbers; far from it – he just used methods in his data analysis that are known to find associations where there are none.

Now, attention is being brought to a broader issue: What kind of system would lead a nutrition researcher down this sketchy path? Some are blaming research culture for making nutrition studies the way they are – not by incentivizing research misconduct per se, but incentivizing flashy findings and a long list of scientific publications.

But if this were the case, all of science would flip-flop constantly. Because all of academic research is incentivized this way.

So instead of looking for answers from the researchers or their broader academic context, which are perhaps not so unique, why not stick to looking at the thing that all nutrition research has in common?

The Human Digestive System

Human bodies are complicated. A lot of working parts respond to a lot of different stimuli in a lot of different ways.

Inge Lindseth is a clinical nutrition physiologist at the Balderklinikken in Oslo, Norway. He and his colleague Marit Zinöcker published a review earlier this year on why ultra-processed foods could cause health problems because of the way they feed our gut microbes.

The review was interesting in terms of what it could mean for what we should eat, but it was also interesting in the way it even approached the question. Lindseth and Zinöcker looked at the ways these foods could impact health from every angle, keeping the whole complex puzzle together instead of picking any individual piece apart.

Lindseth says that was intentional: The puzzle-piece focus of most nutrition research is simply not going to get us anywhere.

“We have seen so little progress in terms of what we tell the public to eat,” he says.  “You could do a million studies more (the old way) and it’s not going to do much,” he later added.

Blame Game

The problem comes from a mentality that there will be a singular answer at the root of all our health problems. Researchers and the public are eager for evidence that says: It’s carbs. Or sugar, or deliciousness, or fat, or portion sizes, or advertising, or availability in low-income communities, or genetics, or any of the other supposed obesity-drivers we hear about.

And so scientists have researched this way, doing epidemiological studies that follow groups of people over time, looking for an association between one specific food or habit and adverse health effects.

But in reality, when trying to study any one of these factors alone, it’s nearly impossible to keep the rest constant. More importantly, even if you could, that’s not how people operate.

It seems obvious. As Lindseth puts it, “I’m realizing that a lot of intelligent people don’t take the time to think.”

The result is nutrition research that’s not very helpful. But to explain unhelpful research that also constantly contradicts itself, there’s one more puzzle piece that’s often overlooked completely.

Personal Gut Bacteria

Nutrition research has ignored a critical component – or as Lindseth and Zinöcker would say, the critical component – of the human digestive system: the microbes in the gut.

They wrote a commentary this summer that suggests the reason epidemiological studies are so variable in their conclusions could be because of the variation in the gut microbes of the research subjects.

The argument is logical: We know that different people have different communities of bacteria living in their guts. We know that different bacteria can eat different things, grow at different rates, and produce different by-products. So how can we compare two peoples’ responses to a certain food group if they started with two fundamentally different digestive systems?

In short, the researchers propose, nutrition studies are so flip-floppy because different gut microbes alter the answers of what are already too-narrowly focused questions.

It would certainly be nice to have something to point a finger at besides researchers drawing hasty conclusions, journalists over-hyping them, or worse, widespread research misconduct.

But, the researchers emphasize, more research is needed before we can be sure about the role of microbes.

Critics will be quick to point out that the reason epidemiological studies are so big, often with hundreds of participants, is in hopes that their large sample size will swamp out any complications from confounding factors like gut bacteria.

Other researchers are skeptical of the relative importance of the gut microbiome at all, and have presented evidence that effects are small in context of other factors.

But above all, Lindseth just seems to hope we’ll start paying attention to the big picture of our health, considering all the pieces together. Preferably while we eat some whole, unprocessed foods.

“The point is, that without a sufficient view of the bigger picture and a real interest in doing hours and hours of contemplation about the possible mechanisms at hand, we are not going to get very far.”

1 free article left
Want More? Get unlimited access for as low as $1.99/month

Already a subscriber?

Register or Log In

1 free articleSubscribe
Discover Magazine Logo
Want more?

Keep reading for as low as $1.99!

Subscribe

Already a subscriber?

Register or Log In

More From Discover
Recommendations From Our Store
Shop Now
Stay Curious
Join
Our List

Sign up for our weekly science updates.

 
Subscribe
To The Magazine

Save up to 40% off the cover price when you subscribe to Discover magazine.

Copyright © 2024 Kalmbach Media Co.