The "Synergistic Core" of the Human Brain?

Are synergistic interactions the fundamental drivers of complex cognition?

Neuroskeptic iconNeuroskeptic
By Neuroskeptic
Sep 26, 2020 5:00 PMSep 28, 2020 3:10 PM
Brain Tree Illustration - Stuart Bradford
(Credit: Stuart Bradford)

Newsletter

Sign up for our email newsletter for the latest science news
 

A new preprint makes a bold claim: that "synergistic interactions are the fundamental drivers of complex human cognition."

What does this mean, and what are the implications for neuroscience?

Authors Andrea Luppi et al. of the University of Cambridge study a new aspect of brain organization: synergy between brain regions. Some networks of the brain are more synergistic than others, and synergistic networks tend to be involved in complex cognition.

So what is synergy? Luppi et al. define it in accordance with the mathematical framework called MMI-PID. Two variables are said to have a synergistic interaction to the extent that the future state of both variables can only be predicted from the past state of both variables together, not from the past state of either variable alone.

As an example, consider two balls moving in space. The two balls might or might not collide. If we know the current trajectory of both balls, we can predict what will happen — including collisions. But if we only know the trajectory of one ball, we can't predict the path of either ball very well, because we wouldn't be able to predict collisions.

In neural terms, if two brain regions have high synergy between them (synergistic interaction), I think the most straightforward interpretation would be that complex two-way interactions are occurring between them — but see later for caveats.

What Luppi et al. do is divide the brain into 232 regions, and then for each pair of regions, calculate the degree of synergy between the two activity timecourses in that pair, based on resting state fMRI data from the Human Connectome Project. For each region they then calculate the average synergy between that region and all of the others.

In the same way, Luppi et al. also examined "redundancy," which is, loosely speaking, the opposite of synergy. Two variables are redundant if both variables provide the same information about the future of the pair.

This is the map of synergy — redundancy. Brain regions in red show high synergy and low redundancy, those in blue have more redundancy.

(Credit: Luppi et al. 2020 bioarXiv)

This map is the core of the preprint. The rest is basically commentary on this, showing how this synergy-redundancy map is similar to other maps such as a map of areas involved in complex cognitive functions, a map of areas showing high synaptic density, gene expression maps, and a map of areas that expanded in humans versus chimpanzees. The implication is that synergy has something to do with higher brain function:

Synergistic interactions are ideally poised to act as a global workspace, allowing the integration of complementary information from across the brain in the service of higher cognitive functions.

This is a very impressive piece of work that integrates a wide range of different datasets. Luppi et al. make a strong case that there's a network of brain areas that tend to participate in synergistic interactions. But what exactly does the synergy mean?

I mentioned before that synergy could be seen as a measure of complex, "emergent" mutual interaction between two brain regions. This is an attractive interpretation, but it is not necessarily the correct one.

For instance, I'm pretty sure that two areas could have a "synergistic interaction," even if neither area was even sending any signals at all. This would happen if both areas were receiving signals from other areas that really were interacting.

To get to the bottom of this, Luppi et al. could extend their analysis to consider more than two regions at once. (They include 232 regions, but only look at interactions involving two at a time, as far as I know). Mathematical tools to do this are available, e.g. multivariate Granger causality (see also).

Such methods could help verify which brain regions are truly driving synergistic interactions. To work out the role of these interactions in cognition, it would be good to apply this same method to fMRI data recorded during cognitive tasks. Does performing a complex task cause an increase in synergistic interactions?

1 free article left
Want More? Get unlimited access for as low as $1.99/month

Already a subscriber?

Register or Log In

1 free articleSubscribe
Discover Magazine Logo
Want more?

Keep reading for as low as $1.99!

Subscribe

Already a subscriber?

Register or Log In

More From Discover
Recommendations From Our Store
Shop Now
Stay Curious
Join
Our List

Sign up for our weekly science updates.

 
Subscribe
To The Magazine

Save up to 40% off the cover price when you subscribe to Discover magazine.

Copyright © 2024 Kalmbach Media Co.