We have completed maintenance on DiscoverMagazine.com and action may be required on your account. Learn More

The Bilingual Brain

Oct 1, 1997 5:00 AMNov 12, 2019 6:40 AM

Newsletter

Sign up for our email newsletter for the latest science news
 

When Karl Kim immigrated to the United States from Korea as a teenager ten years ago, he had a hard time learning English. Now he speaks it fluently, and recently he had a unique opportunity to see how our brains adapt to a second language. Kim is a graduate student in the lab of Joy Hirsch, a neuroscientist at Memorial Sloan-Kettering Cancer Center in New York. He and Hirsch have recently found evidence that children and adults don’t use the same parts of the brain when learning a second language.

The researchers used an instrument called a functional magnetic resonance imager to study the brains of two groups of bilingual people. One group consisted of those who had learned a second language as children. The other consisted of people who, like Kim, learned their second language later in life. When placed inside the mri scanner, which allowed Kim and Hirsch to see which parts of the brain were getting more blood and were thus more active, people from both groups were asked to think about what they had done the day before, first in one language and then the other. (They couldn’t speak out loud, because any movement would disrupt the scanning.)

Kim and Hirsch looked specifically at two language centers in the brain--Broca’s area, in the left frontal part, which is believed to manage speech production, and Wernicke’s area, in the rear of the brain, thought to process the meaning of language. Both groups of people, Kim and Hirsch found, used the same part of Wernicke’s area no matter what language they were speaking. But their use of Broca’s area differed.

People who learned a second language as children used the same region in Broca’s area for both languages. But those who learned a second language later in life made use of a distinct region in Broca’s area for their second language--near the one activated for their native tongue.

How does Hirsch explain this difference? When language is being hard-wired during development, says Hirsch, the brain may intertwine sounds and structures from all languages into the same area. But once that wiring is complete, the management of a new language, with new sounds and structures, must be taken over by a different part of the brain.

A second possibility is simply that we may acquire languages differently as children than we do as adults. If you watch mothers or family members teaching an infant to speak, says Hirsch, it’s very tactile, it’s very auditory, it’s very visual. There are a lot of different inputs. And that’s very different from sitting in a high school class.

1 free article left
Want More? Get unlimited access for as low as $1.99/month

Already a subscriber?

Register or Log In

1 free articleSubscribe
Discover Magazine Logo
Want more?

Keep reading for as low as $1.99!

Subscribe

Already a subscriber?

Register or Log In

More From Discover
Recommendations From Our Store
Shop Now
Stay Curious
Join
Our List

Sign up for our weekly science updates.

 
Subscribe
To The Magazine

Save up to 40% off the cover price when you subscribe to Discover magazine.

Copyright © 2024 Kalmbach Media Co.