We have completed maintenance on DiscoverMagazine.com and action may be required on your account. Learn More

Everything Worth Knowing About ... How We Learn

Amnesiacs, memory champions and rats, oh my!

By Teal Burrell
Jan 20, 2016 12:00 AMMay 21, 2019 5:27 PM
Everything Worth Knowing About ... How We Learn
Syda Productions/Shutterstock

Newsletter

Sign up for our email newsletter for the latest science news
 

Every day, we flood our brains with new information and different experiences, packing even more memories into our vast collection. But how does that process play out? In the past 200 years, psychologists and neuroscientists have worked to learn how our brains learn.

Researchers continue to piece together how the brain forms memory. Here are a few regions thought to be involved:

Evan Oto/Science Source

What We’ve Learned From ...

Henry Molaison’s brain helped spotlight the hippocampus’ role in memory, so it was frozen for future study. In 2009, a team at the University of California, San Diego dissected the brain and created a 3-D model, a vast improvement over the MRI scans performed while Molaison was alive. Diego Mariscal/Brain Observatory/UC San Diego

Those Who Can’t Remember: To treat his epilepsy, Henry Molaison, known for decades as “H.M.” to protect his identity, had parts of his temporal lobe, including the hippocampus, surgically removed from both sides of his brain in 1953. Although the surgery reduced his seizures, he couldn’t form new memories. He remembered experiences and people he met before the operation, but not after. He learned new skills, but never remembered actually practicing them. Molaison’s experience suggested the hippocampus helps form new memories, while long-term memories and subconscious skill memories reside elsewhere in the brain. And Those Who Are Memory Champions: In the 1920s, Solomon Shereshevsky’s extraordinary memory piqued psychologists’ interest. His brain automatically conjured up images for words — blue evoked an image of a person waving a blue flag from a window; seven was a mustachioed man. Although the mental imagery overwhelmed Shereshevsky, it also helped him remember everything he devoted his attention to, such as written letters and lines of poetry in an unfamiliar language.

Solomon Shereshevsky. Luciano Mecacci, Solomon V. Shereshevsky: The Great Russian Mnemonist, Cortex, Volume 49, Issue 8, September 2013, Pages 2260-2263

Memory champions — winners of contests that test feats of recall, like quickly learning the order of stacks of cards — have similar capabilities, thanks to certain mnemonic techniques. Joshua Foer, author of Moonwalking With Einstein, chronicled his attempt to become a memory champion. He concocted visually elaborate stories to memorize the cards’ order, such as Michael Jackson moonwalking (the king of hearts), John Goodman eating a hamburger (king of clubs) or Bill Clinton smoking a cigar (king of diamonds). Foer says these tricks don’t only work for memory champions. “All of our memories are extraordinary,” he says. “If you can cook up a crazy image, really see it in your mind’s eye, it becomes very memorable."

In his quest to become a memory champion, Joshua Foer came up with visually elaborate stories to help him memorize the order of cards within stacks. Christopher Michel

How to Chunk:

Chunking is another helpful memory technique that entails grouping random pieces of information into more meaningful and manageable “chunks.” For example, if you’re trying to remember a string of numbers, break it up into notable dates. (07041031 is Independence Day, then Halloween).

Jay Smith

Making a Memory

Jay Smith

Infantile Amnesia The birth of new hippocampal neurons may help explain infantile amnesia — the fact that adults can’t remember experiences from before age 3. Lots of new neurons get added to hippocampal circuitry at that age, disrupting existing connections and causing us to forget experiences. In adults, new neurons pop up more slowly, but the forgetting continues, just to a lesser degree, and may serve to clear away meaningless and irrelevant information. “Luckily, young kids don’t forget useful skills like walking or talking,” says Paul Frankland of the Hospital for Sick Children in Toronto. “They only forget how they learned those skills.”

Putting a Place to a Face Itzhak Fried at the University of California, Los Angeles, has shown that when patients recall a video clip, their neural networks activate in the same way as when they first saw it. In other studies, Fried actually saw associations forming — neurons that originally fired for celebrities (like Clint Eastwood) began to also fire for landmarks (like the Hollywood sign) after patients saw pictures of the celebrity-landmark pairings. This shows that neural networks can change quickly to associate new information with old memories.

False Memories Without realizing it, we often make inferences to fill in gaps or remember being somewhere we weren’t because we’re so familiar with the story. It’s likely these false memories get reinforced the same way real ones do: During the recall process, the circuit gets fortified, strengthening the inaccuracies. Henry Roediger at Washington University in St. Louis, who studies false memories, says the brain can’t tell the difference between real and false memories, making our fabricated memories seem authentic.

A surgeon drills into a patient’s skull to prepare for deep brain stimulation. BSIP/UIG via Getty Images

Still to Learn:

How to boost memory: Perhaps someday, electrical stimulation could be used to strengthen specific memories. According to a 2014 study, deep brain stimulation, a treatment currently used for Parkinson’s disease, has been shown to spark memories and feelings of déjà vu in a small subset of people when applied to the temporal lobe, where the hippocampus lives.

A History of Learning About Memory

  • 350 B.C. Aristotle writes in De Anima (On the Soul) that people are born with a mind like a blank slate onto which experiences are carved.

  • 1885 German psychologist Herman Ebbinghaus creates about 2,300 nonsense syllables, forces himself to memorize lists of them, and tests how quickly he forgets the lists. He compiles his data into an equation that can be plotted on a graph as a “forgetting curve.” His project launches the study of learning.

  • 1920s Psychologist Karl Lashley is among the first to study learning by testing how rats navigate mazes. Before and after training the rats, he randomly removes different parts of their cortices to see which areas are responsible for remembering the maze. Since many of his lesions disrupt memory, he reasons that memories live throughout the brain, not in only one region.

  • 1930s American neurosurgeon Wilder Penfield pioneers a technique to study the brain during surgery on epilepsy patients: While patients are awake, he stimulates different brain sections and has them report what they see or feel. He discovers that stimulating part of the temporal lobe causes patients to recall forgotten experiences in vivid detail.

  • 1949 Canadian psychologist Donald Hebb proposes that synchronized activity between neurons promotes learning. When one neuron continuously “fires” and activates another, their connection strengthens — hence the common neuroscience phrase, “Neurons that fire together, wire together.”

  • 1950s Studies of “H.M.” demonstrate why the hippocampus is important and that different brain regions store different types of memories.

  • 1973 Terje Lømo, a physiologist in Norway, publishes results revealing that repeated stimulation of one hippocampal neuron leads to an increased response in a neuron connected to it. The connected neuron “learns” the stimulation and remembers it hours later. This phenomenon is called long-term potentiation.

  • 1970s Columbia University neuroscientist Eric Kandel finds that repeatedly triggering sea slugs’ reflexes causes a change in the amount of chemicals released from neurons. This change of the settings controlling chemical release is a mechanism for short-term memory. His work, which earned him the Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine in 2000, also shows long-term memory requires protein synthesis and new connections between neurons.

1 free article left
Want More? Get unlimited access for as low as $1.99/month

Already a subscriber?

Register or Log In

1 free articleSubscribe
Discover Magazine Logo
Want more?

Keep reading for as low as $1.99!

Subscribe

Already a subscriber?

Register or Log In

More From Discover
Recommendations From Our Store
Shop Now
Stay Curious
Join
Our List

Sign up for our weekly science updates.

 
Subscribe
To The Magazine

Save up to 40% off the cover price when you subscribe to Discover magazine.

Copyright © 2024 Kalmbach Media Co.