Planet Earth

Early Signifiers

By Carl ZimmerMay 1, 1995 5:00 AM

Newsletter

Sign up for our email newsletter for the latest science news
 

Sunset on the African savanna, a long, long time ago, and a clan of human ancestors are engaged in a little after-dinner chat. They’re celebrating their good fortune; how often do you stumble across a fresh antelope carcass right next to a patch of ripe berries? A child asks how the adults got the meat off the antelope, so they demonstrate the use of stone blades, explaining how it’s all in the wrist. The conversation is animated, but if you could eavesdrop on it, all you’d hear would be occasional grunts and squeals. The hominids aren’t talking; they’re communicating by means of swift, complex hand gestures--that is, in sign language.

To many paleoanthropologists, that scenario would sound far- fetched. Ever since researchers began to wonder how our ancestors evolved language, they have assumed that language equals speech. But in a recently published book, an anthropologist and two linguists whose specialty is the modern sign language of the deaf challenge this assumption. Language, they claim, did not emerge suddenly in modern humans, as many researchers believe, within the past 50,000 years or so. Instead it evolved gradually, beginning with our earliest hominid and even our primate ancestors. And at first it was a language of gestures rather than words.

There is no direct evidence, of course, that early hominids had any language at all. But anthropologist David Armstrong of Gallaudet University--a Washington, D.C., school for the deaf--and his colleagues have one piece of indirect evidence for their speculation. The size of hominid brains increased dramatically long before Homo sapiens emerged, from 24 cubic inches or so 4 million years ago to an average of 60 cubic inches among Homo erectus, the people on that African savanna. (Human brains range in size from around 60 to 100 cubic inches.) The bigger brain was not simply running a bigger body; the brain-to-body ratio exploded as well, in a way that has not been seen in any other animal lineage. The question is, what were the early hominids doing with those brains? says Armstrong.

Figuring out how to use tools is one possibility. But chimpanzees today, with brains a third the size of ours, use sticks as tools, and the chipped stones of early hominids weren’t much more complicated. Language is a more sensible explanation for swelling brains, says Armstrong; it would have required far more mental capacity, and it would have offered clear advantages to a troop of hunter-gatherers. Yet early hominid language, if it existed, probably couldn’t have been spoken language--because early hominids probably couldn’t speak. Fossils suggest that their throats were like those of chimps: the pharynx, the chamber just above the vocal cords where sounds can be modulated, was too small to make the complex noises necessary for speech.

The notion that hominids didn’t need speech for language--that they used sign language instead--has come up before, but it has never been taken very seriously, in part because sign language itself hasn’t been. Linguists traditionally considered sign language a crude aping of spoken English. That attitude began to change in the 1960s, though, thanks to the efforts of people like linguist William Stokoe, also of Gallaudet, who showed that signing is as linguistically complex as speech. More-recent research has demonstrated that signing draws on many of the same parts of the brain that speech does, and that deaf children learn sign language much as children who can hear learn speech, sign-babbling away. When I first proposed that signing was a language itself and not just a way of representing spoken English, the idea didn’t go down well, says Stokoe. But the changes since then have been just astounding.

The time is thus ripe for a new book, Gesture and the Nature of Language, in which Stokoe, Armstrong, and Sherman Wilcox of the University of New Mexico argue that signing is not only a fully equivalent language but may have been the first language of all. In their scenario, the seeds of sign language were planted in our chimplike ancestors. Primates in general have highly developed vision, and they have specialized bundles of neurons that respond strongly to the sight of moving arms. The primate nervous system may have already been tuned to this sort of stimulus, says Armstrong. In addition, chimpanzees can be taught a crude sort of sign language, suggesting that the brain of our common ancestors was beginning to be capable of language.

But chimps are held back from a full-blown sign language by two limitations: their small brains and their hands. Chimpanzees are knuckle walkers and have relatively long fingers and short thumbs, so the thumb isn’t readily opposable as it is in humans, explains Armstrong. One of the things you notice when you watch films of chimps signing, if you know much about sign language, is that they simply can’t make some of the basic hand shapes. When our ancestors became bipeds, however, their hands quickly took their modern form. That allowed them to wield their primitive tools more skillfully--and it also allowed them to make complex hand gestures. Long before we had a throat fully capable of speech, we had hands fully capable of sign language.

With signing rather than speech as a starting point, one of the central mysteries in the origin of language--the question of how people first came to combine words into sentences--becomes a lot less mysterious. The usual idea is that hominids worked up this tremendous list of words and they finally got so many they had to classify them into categories like nouns and verbs, says Stokoe. Well, who told them how to do that? I don’t know how many vocal signs you’d have to get before the idea would dawn on someone that some of them represented actions and some represented actors and you could put them together.

In sign language, the gap between word and sentence can be bridged naturally in a single sign. The sign for grab in American Sign Language, for example, is one hand closing around an outstretched finger on the other hand. But this gesture can also mean he grabbed it. The moving hand embodies the subject, the movement is the action, and the other hand is the direct object. If an ancient hominid saw a leopard jump on a gazelle, says Stokoe, he might have gone and made a gesture to another hominid that was a motion of the hand like the front paws of the leopard and grabbed his other hand. That doesn’t say leopard, but the leopard jumped on something. You get the relationship of the thing to what it does in one unit. Other aspects of grammar could have emerged in a similarly natural way; facial expressions, for example, are natural adverbs, modifying the actions that are signed.

Once signing had permitted the invention of sentences, though, our ancestors eventually switched to speech. For an animal that uses tools and probably moves around and has to carry things, it’s going to make a lot more sense that the primary communication be vocal, says Armstrong. But perhaps that primordial sign language never really died out. People who have no contact with deaf people use a whole repertoire of manual gestures, and some are identical to the signs in sign language, Armstrong says. We all have some common stock of signs.

While humans may have shifted from signs to speech long ago, Stokoe, Armstrong, and Wilcox (none of whom are deaf) don’t want people to think that they consider modern sign language primitive--just the opposite. One of the motivations of my work, says Stokoe, is to let people know that deaf people are just as bright and able to handle abstract thought as hearing people, given equal education. Prejudice against them because they don’t speak leads to all sorts of bad judgments about deaf people. The fact that we all probably owe a great deal to the era when language began in a gestural state might change some of that.

..SECTWO: ..SECTHREE: ..PQ: ..ITITLE: *** BRS DOCUMENT BOUNDARY *** ..Document-Number: 000000505 ..ISSUE: MAY 1995 ..ISSNO: 199505 ..DEPRT: Animal Watch ..TOPIC: Biology and Medicine ..TITLE: Taking Back the Nest ..AUTHR: Daniel Dunaief ..ADECK:

Historically speaking, the brown-headed cowbird should be called the buffalo bird. This resourceful parasite, which lays its eggs in the nests of other birds, once followed herds of buffalo around the Great Plains, feeding on insects and seeds in the soil churned up by the grazing animals. As Europeans settled North America, however, they replaced forests with pastures full of cows--that is, with cowbird territory. Since cowbirds reproduce ferociously--not having to feed her young, a single female can lay as many as 50 eggs in a breeding season--they soon fanned out across the continent. Today their eggs are routinely found in the nests of a hundred or so species of birds.

Some of those species are in trouble as a result: they’re raising too many baby cowbirds and not enough young of their own. Among the songbirds that have been pushed closer to extinction by cowbirds are the least Bell’s vireo of California, the black-capped vireo of Texas, and the Kirtland’s warbler of Michigan. The situation has forced conservationists into the uncomfortable position of advocating cowbird extermination programs. While we don’t like killing cowbirds, says Jane Griffith, a biological consultant, we do like to hear the songs of endangered species.

On a local level, at least, such programs seem to help. An example is the one that Griffith and her husband, John Griffith, have worked on at Camp Pendleton, a Marine base in Oceanside, California, that is one of the last refuges of the least Bell’s vireo. In the early 1980s, half the vireo nests there were parasitized. By 1994, after some 4,800 cowbirds had been trapped and gassed with carbon monoxide, parasitism had declined to 1 percent. More important, the number of male vireos had increased more than fifteenfold, from 27 in 1981 to 420 in 1993. Similar success has been reported by a cowbird control program at Fort Hood, Texas, which has become a retreat for the black-capped vireo.

Meanwhile, a new threat to songbirds has surfaced--an invasion of another species of cowbird. Since 1985 the shiny cowbird of South America has been sighted in Florida. According to Alexander Cruz, a biologist at the University of Colorado in Boulder, it hasn’t yet been caught parasitizing nests, but that’s just a matter of time. Cruz says the Cuban yellow warbler and the Florida prairie warbler may be particularly vulnerable.

Florida is the cowbird frontier: as the shiny cowbird spreads into the state from the south, the brown-headed cowbird is invading from the north. No one knows what’s going to happen as the two species converge. They may get along just fine--which would be bad news for songbirds. The shiny cowbird has a longer breeding season, and so it may be able to use the nests of early- and late-breeding songbirds that escape its brown- headed cousin. It also seems to favor coastal regions, whereas the brown- headed cowbird is happy inland as well. Cruz thinks the shiny cowbird is likely to spread along the Atlantic and Gulf coasts.

Some conservation biologists, including the Griffiths, believe that more-radical steps must be taken to control cowbirds. They favor killing brown-headed cowbirds not only in places where the birds are parasitizing the nests of endangered species but also at their winter roosting grounds in the southern third of the country. Cowbirds from all over congregate there by the millions and would be easy to kill in large numbers.

But other researchers who oppose such radical measures point out that cowbirds are not the primary threat to endangered songbirds--humans are. the least Bell’s vireo, for example, has lost 95 percent of its habitat--trees and bushes along southwestern riverbanks--to farms and other human uses. Camp Pendleton is one of the few places where it can breed in peace, and in that confined space it is particularly vulnerable to cowbirds.

Restoring habitat, most researchers agree, is the only long-term solution to the songbirds’ woes. It would help them directly, by giving them breeding space, and indirectly, by taking that space from cowbirds. In New England, for instance, the farms established by settlers have been reverting to forest for the past century or so--and there the cowbird population has declined substantially.

1 free article left
Want More? Get unlimited access for as low as $1.99/month

Already a subscriber?

Register or Log In

1 free articleSubscribe
Discover Magazine Logo
Want more?

Keep reading for as low as $1.99!

Subscribe

Already a subscriber?

Register or Log In

More From Discover
Recommendations From Our Store
Shop Now
Stay Curious
Join
Our List

Sign up for our weekly science updates.

 
Subscribe
To The Magazine

Save up to 70% off the cover price when you subscribe to Discover magazine.

Copyright © 2023 Kalmbach Media Co.